Subtitles section Play video Print subtitles OK, so the last time we were here we got maybe a third of the way through this story the story of Pinokio and the transformation of a marionette into something hypothetically real I'm gonna backtrack a few slides and it'll get us into it again so you remember that the blue fairy, so I would say that the benevolent element of mother nature in the schemata that we are going to use to investigate mythology was more or less allowed her entrance because Gepetto was a good guy and he wished for the right thing and so in some sense... here's a way of thinking about that... you know genetic / environmental studies on children's temperament have revealed something quite interesting which is that the shared environment that children have within a family so that would be what's the same about your environment and your brother's environment, the same doesn't have that much effect on your temperament or his temperament 'cause the presumption always was that within a family there is a shared environment, right? and something was common about the environment to every child within that environment but there isn't much of a shared environmental effect on temperament so then you can say, well that makes it appear as though isn't that relevant in relationship to the development of temperament but you could also suggest something else you could suggest that if parenting is occurring properly, the effect of the shared environment should be very close to zero and the reason for that is that you establish an individual relationship with each child and the environment is actually a microenvironment that's composed of your observations of this child and that specific child's interaction with you like to some degree, if there is a shared environment, that means that you're forcing the same principles on every child so my suspicions are, although I don't know this, and the research hasn't been done that in bad families there's a shared environmental effect, but in good families that minimizes so that lets the child's biological predisposition, roughly, manifest itself with support and in some positive manner well, I don't want to extend the analogy too far, but you can imagine that, and this is what this film proposes, if you aim properly in relationship to your child what you're trying to do is to establish an individual relationship and to allow them to move towards whatever their particular expression of individuality happens to be and that's... that would be the same as allowing nature to take its course in some sense at least nature in its positive guise, and that's exactly what happens here the other thing that happens, of course, is that the cricket, for reasons that aren't clear, precisely is knighted by the blue fairy and serves as Pinokio's conscience although he isn't very good at it, which is a very peculiar thing, and quite a marked point that the film is making that that conscience actually has something to learn, too and there's actually a Freudian element to that, you know, because Freud thought of the superego as the internalization, roughly speaking, of the father, and it could be very severe, the superego, so like a really strict father, really tyrannical father inside your head although I think it's better to think about the superego as the internalized representation of society at large mediated to some degree through your parents, 'cause it's not as if your father, even assuming he's tyrannical is the inventor of all those tyrannical rules, he's the propagator of them but he's actually a proxy voice, even if it's just for the harsh side of society, he's the proxy voice for society and because we're social creatures, the utility of having an internal social voice to guide you although, again, you seem to be able to follow it or not follow it, which I also find spectacularly interesting because, obviously if it was an unerring guide, you could just follow it and if it was an unerring guide, you wouldn't need free will either, because you could just act out the dictates of this internal representation that isn't what you do so anyways, the proposition here is that the conscience exists, but it's a relatively flawed entity it needs to be modified as well by nature which is quite interesting, 'cause the blue fairy knights him, 'cause you also might think of the conscience as only something that's socially constructed right, which is the more typical viewpoint, but I don't buy that for a second because I believe firmly, and I believe the Piagetian interpretation of child development more or less bears this out, is that there are parameters within which conscience has to operate and it's sort of like this, it's like, it's the same parameters that govern fair play, we'll say that and so you can say there's fair play within a game, and there's fair play across sets of games and the set of games is pretty much indistinguishable from the actual environment if you think all the things you do as nested games, at some point the spread of that is large enough so that it encompasses everything you do which includes the environment, and so I believe that you're adapted to the set of all possible games, roughly speaking all possible playable games, something like that and that you know the rules for that, which is why, we talked about this a little bit, why you're so good at identifying cheaters we have a module for that, according to the evolutionary psychologists and not only you identify them, but you remember them, it really sticks in your mind and there's other evidence, too, one piece of evidence that I love, well, there's a couple one I would derive from Frans de Waal, who's a famous primatologis, and he studied the prototype morality that emerges in chimpanzees and it's very much nested in their dominance structures you know, because you could think of morality in some sense as the understanding of the rules by which the dominance hierarchy operates, right and so you could say, well, the biggest, ugliest, meanest chimp... and the male dominance hierarchies in chimps seem to be the predominant ones, although the females also have a dominance hierarchy it's not quite so clear in bonobos, which seem to be more female-dominated but in any case, the primary chimp dominance structure is male and you could think, well it's like the caveman chimp who's biggest and toughest who necessarily rules, and who rules longest but that isn't what de Waal found; see, the problem with being... mean, lets say and not negotiating your social landscape, and not trading reciprocal favors is that no matter how powerful you are as an individual, two individuals three quarters your power could do you in and that happens with the chimps fairly regularly; if the guy on top is too tyrannical and doesn't make social connections, then weaker chimps, males, make good social connections and when he's not in such good shape, they take him down, and viciously too de Waal has documented some unbelievably horrendous acts of, let's call it, regicide among the chimpanzee troupes that he studied, mostly in the Arnhem zoo the big troupe there, that's been there a long time but he's very interested in prototypical morality, and here's some other examples of prototypical morality emerging among animals, there's many of them, but one is you know, if two wolves have a dominance dispute, again that would be more likely among the male wolves but it doesn't really matter, they basically display their size, and they growl ferociously and they puff up their hair so they look bigger, and you can see cats do that when they go into fight or flight not only do they puff up, including their tail, but they stand sideways and the reason they do that is because they look bigger right, 'cause they're trying to put up the most intimidating possible front so anyways, if two wolves are going at it, what they're really trying to do is to size each other up and they're trying to scare each other into backing off, fundamentally because, see, the worst-case scenario is like, you're wolf number one, and I'm wolf number two and we tear each other to shreds, but I win, but I'm so damaged after that wolf number three comes in and takes me out so, like, there's a big cost to be paid even for victory in a dominance dispute, if it degenerates into violence and animals, and human beings, but animals in particular, have evolved very, very specific mechanisms to escalate dominance disputes towards violence step by step so that they don't... so that the victor doesn't risk incapacitating himself by winning so what happens with the wolves is that, you know, they growl at each other and posture display, and maybe they even snap at each other but the probability that they're gonna get into a full-fledged fight is pretty low and what happens is, one of the wolves backs off, and flips over and shows his neck and that basically means: "all right, tear it out," and the other wolf says: though of course he doesn't, "well, you're kind of an idiot, and you're not that strong, but we might need you to take down a moose in the future and, you know, despite your patheticness, I won't tear out your throat" and then they've established their dominance position, and then, from then on, at least for some substantial period of time the subordinate wolf gives way to the dominant wolf but at least the subordinate wolf is alive, and, you know, he might be dominant over other wolves and so, everyone in the whole hierarchy has sorted that out either through mock combat or through combat itself and, you know, the low-ranking members aren't in the best possible position, but at least they're not getting their heads torn off every second of their existence so there's even some utility in the stability of the dominance hierarchy for the low-ranking members 'cause at least they're not getting pounded, getting threatened, which is way better I mean it's not good, but it's way better than actual combat and then there's the example of rats, which I love, this is Jaak Panksepp's work and he wrote a book called affective neuroscience, which I highly, highly recommend I have a list of readings, recommended readings on my website it's a brilliant book, and he's a brilliant psychologist, really, one of the top psychologists as far as I'm concerned both theoretically and experimentally, a real genius, he's the guy who discovered that rats laugh when you tickle them they laugh ultrasonically, so you can't actually hear them, but if you record it and slow it down then you can hear them giggling away when you tickle them with an erase, which is sort of like their mother's tongue it's often what lab people use as a substitute for the licking of the little rat by the mother so, and he discovered the paly circuit in mammals, which is like a major deal, right he should get a Nobel prize for that, that's a big deal to discover an entire motivational circuit whose existence no one had really predicted, you know, apart from the fact that obviously mammals play and even lizards maybe, some of them are social lizards, seem to play so, anyhow, what Panksepp observed, and I think this is a brilliant piece of science is that, first of all, juvenile male rats in particular like to rough and tubmle play like to wrestle, and they actually pin each other like little kids do, or like adult wrestlers do they pin their shoulders down, and that basically means you win, and so, OK, so that's pretty cool but what's even cooler, I think, well there's three things, one is: the rats will work for an opportunity to get into an arena where they know that play might occur and so that's one of the scientific ways of testing an animal's motivation, right so imagine you have a starving rat and it knows that it's got food down in the end of a corridoor you can put a little spring on its tail and measure how hard it pulls, and that gives you and idication of its motivational force now, imagine the starving rat that's trying to get to some food, and you have a little spring on its tail, and you waft in some cat odor so now that rat is starving and wants to get out of there, he's going to pull even farther towards the food so getting away plus getting forward are separate motivational systems, and if you can add them together it's real potent and part of the reason why in the future authoring exercise that you guys are gonna do as the class progresses you're asked to outline the place you'd like to end up, which is your desired future and also the place that you could end up if you let everything fall apart so that your anxiety chases you and your approach systems pull you forward you're maximally motivated then, and it's important, because otherwise you can be afraid of pursuing the things you wanna pursue right, and that's very common, and so then the fear inhibits you as the promise pulls you forward but it makes you weak, because you're afraid; you wanna get your fear behind you, pushing you and so what you wanna be is more afraid of not pursuing your goals than you are of pursuing them it's very, very helpful; and lots of times in life, and this is something really worth knowing you know, and this is one of the advantages to being an autonomous adult you don't get to pick the best thing, you get to pick your poison you have two bad choices, and you get to pick which one you're willing to suffer through and every choice has a bit of that element in it, and so, if you know that it's really freeing because otherwise you torture yourself by thinking: "well, maybe there's a good solution to this, compared to the bad solution" it's like, no, no, sometime's there's just risky solution 1, and risky solution 2 and sometimes both of them are really bad, but you at least get to pick which one you're willing to suffer through and that's... that actually makes quite a bit of difference, because you're also facing it voluntarily then instead of it chasing you, and that is an entirely different psychophysiological response challenge vs threat, it's not the same, even if the magnitude of the problem is the same