Placeholder Image

Subtitles section Play video

  • Youre a construction worker. Youre workingjob with a crew of six, building a small railroad  

  • station. Youve been on the job for several  days. By the end of the day today, youll be  

  • done. Youre excited to go home and enjoycouple days off with your friends and family

  • Everything is going normally, beginning to  wrap up, when suddenly, you hear a loud crack.  

  • A pillar on the front of the station has brokencausing the station’s roof to collapse sideways,  

  • pulling the rest of the building’s infrastructure  out and toward you. You try to get out of the way,  

  • but you can’t. You and four co-workers become  pinned on the train tracks by the debris.  

  • You can’t barely move. You try to maintain your  composure, but the feeling of panic begins to set  

  • in, quickly exasperated by the bellowing yells  of your co-workers’. Some of them are injured.  

  • You become confused when suddenly their yells  go quiet. There’s a vibrational hum in the  

  • distance that replaces them. Then, a train  horn. The yells pick back up, now changed  

  • from the sound of pain to the sound of terrorCloser and closer you hear and feel the train  

  • come. This is it, you think to yourself. You hear one of your co-workers yelling,  

  • Switch the track! Switch the track!” You remember there’s a manned control  

  • booth on the bridge about forty yards behind  you. Inside, the attendant can push a button  

  • that will switch the tracks and divert  the train away right before it hits you

  • You join in on the now collective  chant to change the tracks

  • After a moment passes, you hear someone  else yelling near you but not with you

  • It’s your other coworker and good  friend. He is pinned on the other track

  • The person in the control booth sees what’s  happening. They saw the whole thing and called  

  • for help immediately, but it won’t get here  in time. They watch the oncoming train get  

  • closer and closer. They have a decision to  make. Choose to switch the track and have  

  • the train kill one person or do nothing and  let the train kill a total of five people

  • You feel the vibration of the train increase  as you consider your whole life—a tapestry of  

  • experiences and feelings; relationships  and achievements; desires and hopes.  

  • You plead for it to continue. You  don’t want to die. No one here does

  • The person in the booth is thinking intensely  about what to do. They have about five  

  • seconds to decide. 5, 4, 3, 2, 1. 

  • This dilemma is a version of the thought  experiment known as the trolley problem.  

  • Originally posed by philosopher Philippa  Foot in 1967 and later advanced in 1976 by  

  • Judith Jarvis Thomson, the trolley problem has  become an increasingly popular and sometimes  

  • comical category of thought experiments that  challenge our most basic moral intuitions and  

  • show us how unclear questions about ethics  and decision making can easily become

  • To explore this further, imagine  for a moment now that you are the  

  • one in the control booth. What would you do? Would you do nothing, avoiding involving yourself,  

  • but allowing five people to die that  you could have otherwise saved; or,  

  • would you switch the tracks and, in some sensecause one person to die but save the other five

  • For most people, the choice is clear enoughIn a variety of different surveys, about 90%  

  • of respondents claim that in this situationthey would flip the switch and divert the train.  

  • Afterall, five people dying is worse than oneand most people want to do the least worst thing.  

  • This way of thinking follows from one of the most  influential and commonly held ethical theories  

  • known as utilitarianism, originally developed in  the 18th century, which describes decisions and  

  • actions as moral when their outcomes result in  the greatest amount good (pleasure, happiness,  

  • or well-being) and the minimal amount of bad  (pain or suffering). Alternatively, however,  

  • people who disagree with this choice might  follow the ethical theory known as deontology,  

  • which argues that decisions and actions are  moral solely based on whether or not they  

  • follow defined rules and principles, and not  based on their consequences. In other words,  

  • for example, the act of killing someone under  any circumstances, even if by not killing them,  

  • you are essentially killing more people, is and  will always be wrong on principle. And therefore,  

  • since flipping the switch involves you  committing the act of killing someone,  

  • doing so is morally wrong. However, this raises  the following question: what is the difference  

  • between allowing and causing? Is there any? By  simply observing the event, understanding it,  

  • and possessing the ability to easily change  and improve it, are you not already involved?  

  • Is there a difference in involvement between  observation and inaction verses observation  

  • and action? Isn’t choosing to not be involvedchoice that is already involved? No one can choose  

  • to not choose and no one can opt out of what they  already are a part of. What if there was no one  

  • on the other track? Wouldn’t allowing the five  people to die clearly be the wrong thing to do

  • Even at this level, in just the original, basic  version, clearly it isn’t 100% obvious what is  

  • right and how we ought to treat the situation. And  it only gets far less obvious with subtle changes

  • What if we change the numbers? What if there  are fifty people on the main track and ten on  

  • the other? How about five thousand on the main  track and one thousand on the other? Now, if you  

  • switch the tracks, you will direct the death of  one thousand people, but you will also save five  

  • thousand. The proportions are exactly the same  as the original, and therefore the logic should  

  • also be the same, but it seems to feel a lot  different knowing that you will at least be partly  

  • responsible for the death of one thousand people. What if we change the proportions now? On the main  

  • track, there are five people. On the otherthere are two. How about three on the other,  

  • or four? Again, from a purely utilitarian and  seemingly rational perspective, four is still  

  • less bad than five, and so, switching the  tracks is still seemingly the right choice.  

  • But yet, there almost seems to be a point at which  the difference in people between the tracks feels  

  • more negligible than the intervention itselfIs saving one more person worth the weight of  

  • responsibility you will feel in that decisionWhat about your own personal suffering and  

  • trauma as a result of this? How do we weigh  that against the delta of one person’s life

  • Now, let’s make it even harder. What if  there are five people on the main track,  

  • and only one on the other, but the one is someone  you love. A family member, a partner, a friend,  

  • a child. What do you do then? Your impulse is  most likely not to switch the tracks. That is,  

  • of course, very understandable. But is  it right? If, in the original version,  

  • you believe you should switch the tracksthe only difference here is your emotional  

  • attachment to the one person. But the morality  and logic of the situation does not change  

  • otherwise. Even if in the original version  you believe you should not switch the tracks,  

  • just put your loved one in the group of five on  the main track and consider the same problem.  

  • Is your emotional involvement enough to  compromise your morality? Presumably,  

  • the majority of people would say yes and do the  same. But then, we would have to also agree that  

  • morality is flexible based on our emotions  and not based on our consistency of logic

  • How much of ethics is really just emotionanyway? The meta-ethical theory known as  

  • emotivism argues that ethical statements never  express truths or falsehoods, but rather,  

  • merely our emotional states, influenced by things  like our cultural and experiential backgrounds,  

  • moods, personalities, and so on. If this is  true, how does that effect things in general

  • Let’s return to the bridge in the original  example. Again, five people are stuck on the  

  • train tracks and a small train is coming. This  time, however, there is no control booth, button,  

  • switch, or any second track at all. You are just  a random pedestrian watching the event unfold  

  • from the bridge. Near you, however, at the edge of  the bridge, stands a large man also observing the  

  • event unfold. He is massivemorbidly obese. You  don’t know any of the people on track. You don’t  

  • know the obese man. But you do know that if you  push the man off the bridge in front of the train,  

  • he will cause the train to stop in timekilling him but saving the other five people

  • Do you do it? In the same surveys mentioned earlier where 90%  

  • of respondents claimed that they would switch the  track by a lever or button, less than 10% claimed  

  • that they would push the man off the bridge. But  yet, it’s the same outcome, same number of people,  

  • same relationship with the people involvedand so on. Everything else is exactly the  

  • same. Neither the man on the alternative track in  the original scenario nor the man on the bridge  

  • in this scenario chose to be in the situationThis obese man is no more or less involved in  

  • the unfolding of events, and they would both be  just as safe if you weren’t there. But yet, you  

  • likely feel differently in this situation, simply  because it feels more personal. Emotionally,  

  • you are affected enough to not do what you  already (likely) agreed was logical and moral

  • For one last twist, let’s imagine a different  person is in this same situation on the bridge  

  • making the decision of whether or not to push  the man. This person, however, is psychopathic,  

  • and they choose to do whatever you chose to  do, but they do it because they want to feel  

  • personally involved in killing someone or they  want to see more people die. In either case,  

  • does their intention change the morality  of the situation? Would they be more or  

  • less moral than someone who chooses to do the  opposite if the psychopaths actions results in  

  • the same outcome of what you believe is moralTaking the utilitarian view, the psychopathic  

  • person would have done the more moral thing. But  who would you want to be alone in a room with

  • Let’s say that they did do the more moral thingbut they are an immoral person, justifiably worth  

  • condemning. But what happens if six weeks laterthey go to a neurologist after having experienced  

  • months of chronic headaches, and it is discovered  that they have a tumor in their brain that had  

  • caused brain lesions in areas responsible for  morality and value-based decision-making? The  

  • tumor has directly caused their behavior and  decision making to change in ways that they  

  • had no control over. How do we perceive them  now? Are they morally responsible for their  

  • behavior? What if the same person didn’t have  a tumor but was horribly abused psychologically  

  • and physically as a child, which directly  caused a response that led to this behavior.  

  • They would not have chosen or controlled this  anymore than a tumor. Is there a difference?  

  • Is anyone truly morally responsible for  who they become and the choices they are  

  • led to make if they are by causes they never  chose or controlled to begin with? Of course,  

  • the individual still needs to be dealt with and  taken out of societyrehabilitated if possible.  

  • But just because you can’t take the tumor outdoes not make the person any less of a victim

  • Ultimately, the trolley problems aren’t just  interesting thought experiments and goofy  

  • memes. They show us that what we think is right  or wrong is often based on a lot more than just  

  • rational evaluations of pros and cons, good and  bad. They reveal to us that decisions and actions  

  • are often a consequence of a complex array of  intentions and beliefs, all puppeteered by a  

  • backstory of cause-and-effect in a chaotic  system comprised of incalculable variables.  

  • The point of these questions isn’t to  trick the mind, but to demonstrate just  

  • how complicated and unclear matters of ethical  decision making sometimes are and can be

  • Right now, in this moment of history, we are  entering new realms of moral understanding and new  

  • conditions of societal reality. What rights might  we owe our animal cousins? How should we program  

  • AI, self-driving vehicles, and other technologies  to make decisions on their own about things like,  

  • for example, whether or not a self-driving  car should hit one person in order to avoid  

  • five? How should we choose when all choices  are bad? How will we? How should we update  

  • laws and justice systems and punishments as we  venture out further into these new realms of  

  • understanding and society? In the words of Peter  Singer, “What one generation finds ridiculous,  

  • the next accepts; and the third shudders  when it looks back on what the first did.” 

  • It is both a wonder and an incredible feat that  we have come as far as we have as a society and  

  • as a speciesto understand and agree, at scalewhat it means to at least try to be moral, and  

  • to do our best every day to behave accordinglyTo attempt uphold peace, and order, and reason,  

  • and understanding. Of course, there is so much  chaos, disdain, and misalignment in the world. Our  

  • immorality is always lurking in each moment and  each stage of history. But there is also so much  

  • compassion, so much kindness and desire to treat  other people and things right, as best we can

  • We have come so far. We must not forget this.  

  • But we have so much farther still to  go. We must not forget this either.

Youre a construction worker. Youre workingjob with a crew of six, building a small railroad  

Subtitles and vocabulary

Click the word to look it up Click the word to find further inforamtion about it