Subtitles section Play video
-
So why do you think the rich should pay more in taxes?
-
Why did you buy the latest iPhone?
-
Why did you pick your current partner?
-
And why did so many people vote for Donald Trump?
-
What were the reasons, why did they do it?
-
So we ask this kind of question all the time,
-
and we expect to get an answer.
-
And when being asked, we expect ourselves to know the answer,
-
to simply tell why we did as we did.
-
But do we really know why?
-
So when you say that you prefer George Clooney to Tom Hanks,
-
due to his concern for the environment,
-
is that really true?
-
So you can be perfectly sincere and genuinely believe
-
that this is the reason that drives your choice,
-
but to me, it may still feel like something is missing.
-
As it stands, due to the nature of subjectivity,
-
it is actually very hard to ever prove that people are wrong about themselves.
-
So I'm an experimental psychologist,
-
and this is the problem we've been trying to solve in our lab.
-
So we wanted to create an experiment
-
that would allow us to challenge what people say about themselves,
-
regardless of how certain they may seem.
-
But tricking people about their own mind is hard.
-
So we turned to the professionals.
-
The magicians.
-
So they're experts at creating the illusion of a free choice.
-
So when they say, "Pick a card, any card,"
-
the only thing you know is that your choice is no longer free.
-
So we had a few fantastic brainstorming sessions
-
with a group of Swedish magicians,
-
and they helped us create a method
-
in which we would be able to manipulate the outcome of people's choices.
-
This way we would know when people are wrong about themselves,
-
even if they don't know this themselves.
-
So I will now show you a short movie showing this manipulation.
-
So it's quite simple.
-
The participants make a choice,
-
but I end up giving them the opposite.
-
And then we want to see: How did they react, and what did they say?
-
So it's quite simple, but see if you can spot the magic going on.
-
And this was shot with real participants, they don't know what's going on.
-
(Video) Petter Johansson: Hi, my name's Petter.
-
Woman: Hi, I'm Becka.
-
PJ: I'm going to show you pictures like this.
-
And you'll have to decide which one you find more attractive.
-
Becka: OK.
-
PJ: And then sometimes, I will ask you why you prefer that face.
-
Becka: OK.
-
PJ: Ready? Becka: Yeah.
-
PJ: Why did you prefer that one?
-
Becka: The smile, I think.
-
PJ: Smile.
-
Man: One on the left.
-
Again, this one just struck me.
-
Interesting shot.
-
Since I'm a photographer, I like the way it's lit and looks.
-
Petter Johansson: But now comes the trick.
-
(Video) Woman 1: This one.
-
PJ: So they get the opposite of their choice.
-
And let's see what happens.
-
Woman 2: Um ...
-
I think he seems a little more innocent than the other guy.
-
Man: The one on the left.
-
I like her smile and contour of the nose and face.
-
So it's a little more interesting to me, and her haircut.
-
Woman 3: This one.
-
I like the smirky look better.
-
PJ: You like the smirky look better?
-
(Laughter)
-
Woman 3: This one.
-
PJ: What made you choose him?
-
Woman 3: I don't know, he looks a little bit like the Hobbit.
-
(Laughter)
-
PJ: And what happens in the end
-
when I tell them the true nature of the experiment?
-
Yeah, that's it. I just have to ask a few questions.
-
Man: Sure.
-
PJ: What did you think of this experiment, was it easy or hard?
-
Man: It was easy.
-
PJ: During the experiments,
-
I actually switched the pictures three times.
-
Was this anything you noticed?
-
Man: No. I didn't notice any of that.
-
PJ: Not at all? Man: No.
-
Switching the pictures as far as ...
-
PJ: Yeah, you were pointing at one of them but I actually gave you the opposite.
-
Man: The opposite one. OK, when you --
-
No. Shows you how much my attention span was.
-
(Laughter)
-
PJ: Did you notice that sometimes during the experiment
-
I switched the pictures?
-
Woman 2: No, I did not notice that.
-
PJ: You were pointing at one, but then I gave you the other one.
-
No inclination of that happening?
-
Woman 2: No.
-
Woman 2: I did not notice.
-
(Laughs)
-
PJ: Thank you.
-
Woman 2: Thank you.
-
PJ: OK, so as you probably figured out now,
-
the trick is that I have two cards in each hand,
-
and when I hand one of them over,
-
the black one kind of disappears into the black surface on the table.
-
So using pictures like this,
-
normally not more than 20 percent of the participants detect these tries.
-
And as you saw in the movie,
-
when in the end we explain what's going on,
-
they're very surprised and often refuse to believe the trick has been made.
-
So this shows that this effect is quite robust and a genuine effect.
-
But if you're interested in self-knowledge, as I am,
-
the more interesting bit is,
-
OK, so what did they say when they explained these choices?
-
So we've done a lot of analysis
-
of the verbal reports in these experiments.
-
And this graph simply shows
-
that if you compare what they say in a manipulated trial
-
with a nonmanipulated trial,
-
that is when they explain a normal choice they've made
-
and one where we manipulated the outcome,
-
we find that they are remarkably similar.
-
So they are just as emotional, just as specific,
-
and they are expressed with the same level of certainty.
-
So the strong conclusion to draw from this
-
is that if there are no differences
-
between a real choice and a manipulated choice,
-
perhaps we make things up all the time.
-
But we've also done studies
-
where we try to match what they say with the actual faces.
-
And then we find things like this.
-
So here, this male participant, he preferred the girl to the left,
-
he ended up with the one to the right.
-
And then, he explained his choice like this.
-
"She is radiant.
-
I would rather have approached her at the bar than the other one.
-
And I like earrings."
-
And whatever made him choose the girl on the left to begin with,
-
it can't have been the earrings,
-
because they were actually sitting on the girl on the right.
-
So this is a clear example of a post hoc construction.
-
So they just explained the choice afterwards.
-
So what this experiment shows is,
-
OK, so if we fail to detect that our choices have been changed,
-
we will immediately start to explain them in another way.
-
And what we also found
-
is that the participants often come to prefer the alternative,
-
that they were led to believe they liked.
-
So if we let them do the choice again,
-
they will now choose the face they had previously rejected.
-
So this is the effect we call "choice blindness."
-
And we've done a number of different studies --
-
we've tried consumer choices,
-
choices based on taste and smell and even reasoning problems.
-
But what you all want to know is of course
-
does this extend also to more complex, more meaningful choices?
-
Like those concerning moral and political issues.
-
So the next experiment, it needs a little bit of a background.
-
So in Sweden, the political landscape
-
is dominated by a left-wing and a right-wing coalition.
-
And the voters may move a little bit between the parties within each coalition,
-
but there is very little movement between the coalitions.
-
And before each elections,
-
the newspapers and the polling institutes
-
put together what they call "an election compass"
-
which consists of a number of dividing issues
-
that sort of separates the two coalitions.
-
Things like if tax on gasoline should be increased
-
or if the 13 months of paid parental leave
-
should be split equally between the two parents
-
in order to increase gender equality.
-
So, before the last Swedish election,
-
we created an election compass of our own.
-
So we walked up to people in the street
-
and asked if they wanted to do a quick political survey.
-
So first we had them state their voting intention
-
between the two coalitions.
-
Then we asked them to answer 12 of these questions.
-
They would fill in their answers,
-
and we would ask them to discuss,
-
so OK, why do you think tax on gas should be increased?
-
And we'd go through the questions.
-
Then we had a color coded template
-
that would allow us to tally their overall score.
-
So this person would have one, two, three, four
-
five, six, seven, eight, nine scores to the left,
-
so he would lean to the left, basically.
-
And in the end, we also had them fill in their voting intention once more.
-
But of course, there was also a trick involved.
-
So first, we walked up to people,
-
we asked them about their voting intention
-
and then when they started filling in,
-
we would fill in a set of answers going in the opposite direction.
-
We would put it under the notepad.
-
And when we get the questionnaire,
-
we would simply glue it on top of the participant's own answer.
-
So there, it's gone.
-
And then we would ask about each of the questions:
-
How did you reason here?
-
And they'll state the reasons,
-
together we will sum up their overall score.
-
And in the end, they will state their voting intention again.
-
So what we find first of all here,
-
is that very few of these manipulations are detected.
-
And they're not detected in the sense that they realize,
-
"OK, you must have changed my answer,"
-
it was more the case that,
-
"OK, I must've misunderstood the question the first time I read it.
-
Can I please change it?"
-
And even if a few of these manipulations were changed,
-
the overall majority was missed.
-
So we managed to switch 90 percent of the participants' answers
-
from left to right, right to left, their overall profile.
-
And what happens then when they are asked to motivate their choices?
-
And here we find much more interesting verbal reports
-
than compared to the faces.
-
People say things like this, and I'll read it to you.
-
So, "Large-scale governmental surveillance of email and internet traffic
-
ought to be permissible as means to combat international crime and terrorism."
-
"So you agree to some extent with this statement." "Yes."
-
"So how did you reason here?"
-
"Well, like, as it is so hard to get at international crime and terrorism,
-
I think there should be those kinds of tools."
-
And then the person remembers an argument from the newspaper in the morning.
-
"Like in the newspaper today,
-
it said they can like, listen to mobile phones from prison,
-
if a gang leader tries to continue his crimes from inside.
-
And I think it's madness that we have so little power
-
that we can't stop those things
-
when we actually have the possibility to do so."
-
And then there's a little bit back and forth in the end:
-
"I don't like that they have access to everything I do,
-
but I still think it's worth it in the long run."
-
So, if you didn't know that this person
-
just took part in a choice blindness experiment,
-
I don't think you would question
-
that this is the true attitude of that person.
-
And what happens in the end, with the voting intention?
-
What we find -- that one is also clearly affected by the questionnaire.
-
So we have 10 participants
-
shifting from left to right or from right to left.
-
We have another 19 that go from clear voting intention
-
to being uncertain.
-
Some go from being uncertain to clear voting intention.
-
And then there is a number of participants staying uncertain throughout.
-
And that number is interesting
-
because if you look at what the polling institutes say