Subtitles section Play video
-
I'd like to invite you to close your eyes.
-
Imagine yourself standing
-
outside the front door of your home.
-
I'd like you to notice the color of the door,
-
the material that it's made out of.
-
Now visualize a pack of overweight nudists on bicycles.
-
They are competing in a naked bicycle race,
-
and they are headed straight for your front door.
-
I need you to actually see this.
-
They are pedaling really hard, they're sweaty,
-
they're bouncing around a lot.
-
And they crash straight into the front door of your home.
-
Bicycles fly everywhere, wheels roll past you,
-
spokes end up in awkward places.
-
Step over the threshold of your door
-
into your foyer, your hallway, whatever's on the other side,
-
and appreciate the quality of the light.
-
The light is shining down on Cookie Monster.
-
Cookie Monster is waving at you
-
from his perch on top of a tan horse.
-
It's a talking horse.
-
You can practically feel his blue fur tickling your nose.
-
You can smell the oatmeal raisin cookie that he's about to shovel into his mouth.
-
Walk past him. Walk past him into your living room.
-
In your living room, in full imaginative broadband,
-
picture Britney Spears.
-
She is scantily clad, she's dancing on your coffee table,
-
and she's singing "Hit Me Baby One More Time."
-
And then follow me into your kitchen.
-
In your kitchen, the floor has been paved over with a yellow brick road
-
and out of your oven are coming towards you
-
Dorothy, the Tin Man,
-
the Scarecrow and the Lion from "The Wizard of Oz,"
-
hand-in-hand skipping straight towards you.
-
Okay. Open your eyes.
-
I want to tell you about a very bizarre contest
-
that is held every spring in New York City.
-
It's called the United States Memory Championship.
-
And I had gone to cover this contest a few years back
-
as a science journalist
-
expecting, I guess, that this was going to be
-
like the Superbowl of savants.
-
This was a bunch of guys and a few ladies,
-
widely varying in both age and hygienic upkeep.
-
(Laughter)
-
They were memorizing hundreds of random numbers,
-
looking at them just once.
-
They were memorizing the names of dozens and dozens and dozens of strangers.
-
They were memorizing entire poems in just a few minutes.
-
They were competing to see who could memorize
-
the order of a shuffled pack of playing cards the fastest.
-
I was like, this is unbelievable.
-
These people must be freaks of nature.
-
And I started talking to a few of the competitors.
-
This is a guy called Ed Cook
-
who had come over from England
-
where he had one of the best trained memories.
-
And I said to him, "Ed, when did you realize
-
that you were a savant?"
-
And Ed was like, "I'm not a savant.
-
In fact, I have just an average memory.
-
Everybody who competes in this contest
-
will tell you that they have just an average memory.
-
We've all trained ourselves
-
to perform these utterly miraculous feats of memory
-
using a set of ancient techniques,
-
techniques invented 2,500 years ago in Greece,
-
the same techniques that Cicero had used
-
to memorize his speeches,
-
that medieval scholars had used to memorize entire books."
-
And I was like, "Whoa. How come I never heard of this before?"
-
And we were standing outside the competition hall,
-
and Ed, who is a wonderful, brilliant,
-
but somewhat eccentric English guy,
-
says to me, "Josh, you're an American journalist.
-
Do you know Britney Spears?"
-
I'm like, "What? No. Why?"
-
"Because I really want to teach Britney Spears
-
how to memorize the order of a shuffled pack of playing cards
-
on U.S. national television.
-
It will prove to the world that anybody can do this."
-
(Laughter)
-
I was like, "Well I'm not Britney Spears,
-
but maybe you could teach me.
-
I mean, you've got to start somewhere, right?"
-
And that was the beginning of a very strange journey for me.
-
I ended up spending the better part of the next year
-
not only training my memory,
-
but also investigating it,
-
trying to understand how it works,
-
why it sometimes doesn't work
-
and what its potential might be.
-
I met a host of really interesting people.
-
This is a guy called E.P.
-
He's an amnesic who had, very possibly,
-
the very worst memory in the world.
-
His memory was so bad
-
that he didn't even remember he had a memory problem,
-
which is amazing.
-
And he was this incredibly tragic figure,
-
but he was a window into the extent
-
to which our memories make us who we are.
-
The other end of the spectrum: I met this guy.
-
This is Kim Peek.
-
He was the basis for Dustin Hoffman's character in the movie "Rain Man."
-
We spent an afternoon together
-
in the Salt Lake City Public Library memorizing phone books,
-
which was scintillating.
-
(Laughter)
-
And I went back and I read a whole host of memory treatises,
-
treatises written 2,000-plus years ago
-
in Latin in Antiquity
-
and then later in the Middle Ages.
-
And I learned a whole bunch of really interesting stuff.
-
One of the really interesting things that I learned
-
is that once upon a time,
-
this idea of having a trained, disciplined, cultivated memory
-
was not nearly so alien as it would seem to us to be today.
-
Once upon a time, people invested in their memories,
-
in laboriously furnishing their minds.
-
Over the last few millennia
-
we've invented a series of technologies --
-
from the alphabet to the scroll
-
to the codex, the printing press, photography,
-
the computer, the smartphone --
-
that have made it progressively easier and easier
-
for us to externalize our memories,
-
for us to essentially outsource
-
this fundamental human capacity.
-
These technologies have made our modern world possible,
-
but they've also changed us.
-
They've changed us culturally,
-
and I would argue that they've changed us cognitively.
-
Having little need to remember anymore,
-
it sometimes seems like we've forgotten how.
-
One of the last places on Earth
-
where you still find people passionate about this idea
-
of a trained, disciplined, cultivated memory
-
is at this totally singular memory contest.
-
It's actually not that singular,
-
there are contests held all over the world.
-
And I was fascinated, I wanted to know how do these guys do it.
-
A few years back a group of researchers at University College London
-
brought a bunch of memory champions into the lab.
-
They wanted to know:
-
Do these guys have brains
-
that are somehow structurally, anatomically different from the rest of ours?
-
The answer was no.
-
Are they smarter than the rest of us?
-
They gave them a bunch of cognitive tests,
-
and the answer was not really.
-
There was however one really interesting and telling difference
-
between the brains of the memory champions
-
and the control subjects that they were comparing them to.
-
When they put these guys in an fMRI machine,
-
scanned their brains
-
while they were memorizing numbers and people's faces and pictures of snowflakes,
-
they found that the memory champions
-
were lighting up different parts of the brain
-
than everyone else.
-
Of note, they were using, or they seemed to be using,
-
a part of the brain that's involved in spatial memory and navigation.
-
Why? And is there something the rest of us can learn from this?
-
The sport of competitive memorizing
-
is driven by a kind of arms race
-
where every year somebody comes up
-
with a new way to remember more stuff more quickly,
-
and then the rest of the field has to play catchup.
-
This is my friend Ben Pridmore,
-
three-time world memory champion.
-
On his desk in front of him
-
are 36 shuffled packs of playing cards
-
that he is about to try to memorize in one hour,
-
using a technique that he invented and he alone has mastered.
-
He used a similar technique
-
to memorize the precise order
-
of 4,140 random binary digits
-
in half an hour.
-
Yeah.
-
And while there are a whole host of ways
-
of remembering stuff in these competitions,
-
everything, all of the techniques that are being used,
-
ultimately come down to a concept
-
that psychologists refer to as elaborative encoding.
-
And it's well illustrated by a nifty paradox
-
known as the Baker/baker paradox,
-
which goes like this:
-
If I tell two people to remember the same word,
-
if I say to you,
-
"Remember that there is a guy named Baker."
-
That's his name.
-
And I say to you, "Remember that there is a guy who is a baker."
-
And I come back to you at some point later on,
-
and I say, "Do you remember that word
-
that I told you a while back?
-
Do you remember what it was?"
-
The person who was told his name is Baker
-
is less likely to remember the same word
-
than the person was told his job is that he is a baker.
-
Same word, different amount of remembering; that's weird.
-
What's going on here?
-
Well the name Baker doesn't actually mean anything to you.
-
It is entirely untethered
-
from all of the other memories floating around in your skull.
-
But the common noun baker,
-
we know bakers.
-
Bakers wear funny white hats.
-
Bakers have flour on their hands.
-
Bakers smell good when they come home from work.
-
Maybe we even know a baker.
-
And when we first hear that word,
-
we start putting these associational hooks into it
-
that make it easier to fish it back out at some later date.
-
The entire art of what is going on
-
in these memory contests
-
and the entire art of remembering stuff better in everyday life
-
is figuring out ways to transform capital B Bakers
-
into lower-case B bakers --
-
to take information that is lacking in context,
-
in significance, in meaning
-
and transform it in some way
-
so that it becomes meaningful
-
in the light of all the other things that you have in your mind.
-
One of the more elaborate techniques for doing this
-
dates back 2,500 years to Ancient Greece.
-
It came to be known as the memory palace.
-
The story behind its creation goes like this:
-
There was a poet called Simonides
-
who was attending a banquet.
-
He was actually the hired entertainment,
-
because back then if you wanted to throw a really slamming party,
-
you didn't hire a D.J., you hired a poet.
-
And he stands up, delivers his poem from memory, walks out the door,
-
and at the moment he does, the banquet hall collapses,
-
kills everybody inside.
-
It doesn't just kill everybody,
-
it mangles the bodies beyond all recognition.
-
Nobody can say who was inside,
-
nobody can say where they were sitting.
-
The bodies can't be properly buried.
-
It's one tragedy compounding another.
-
Simonides, standing outside,
-
the sole survivor amid the wreckage,
-
closes his eyes and has this realization,
-
which is that in his mind's eye,
-
he can see where each of the guests at the banquet had been sitting.
-
And he takes the relatives by the hand
-
and guides them each to their loved ones amid the wreckage.
-
What Simonides figured out at that moment
-
is something that I think we all kind of intuitively know,
-
which is that, as bad as we are
-
at remembering names and phone numbers